中天是什么意思| 给孩子测骨龄应该挂什么科| 病毒性结膜炎用什么眼药水| 喉部有异物感是什么病| 法令纹上的痣代表什么| 甘油三酯高什么原因| 甄嬛传什么时候上映的| 荷叶又什么又什么| 什么是正太| 睡醒后口苦是什么原因| 关羽使用的武器是什么| 什么的娃娃| 看灰指甲去医院挂什么科| 咖啡豆是什么动物粪便| 晚上总是睡不着觉是什么原因| 10.1是什么星座| 怀孕第一个月最怕什么| 海底有什么生物| kgs是什么单位| 98年属虎的是什么命| 嘴唇正常颜色是什么样| 甲状腺炎吃什么药| 琪五行属什么| 小二阳是什么意思| 宝宝有口臭是什么原因引起的| 良人什么意思| 什么而不舍| 什么水果最有营养| 梳子断了有什么预兆| 颈椎病吃什么药最好| 梦到怀孕生孩子是什么意思| 木薯粉是什么东西| 亲故是什么意思| 中超是什么| 脉是什么意思| 1999年属兔的是什么命| 抗核小体抗体阳性说明什么| 皮肤挂什么科| 3月10日是什么星座| 四肢发麻是什么原因| 晚年是什么意思| 口臭什么原因引起的| 太平间是什么意思| 黄鼠狼进屋是什么兆头| 经期不能吃什么| 1955年出生属什么| babyface是什么意思| 鳜鱼是什么鱼| 宫颈多发潴留囊肿是什么意思| 公安局局长什么级别| 为什么海水是咸的| 积水是什么意思| 一什么鱼| dew是什么意思| 韧带损伤挂什么科| 骑单车锻炼什么好处| xo是什么酒| 包租婆是什么意思| 西藏有什么特产| 118什么意思| 是什么符号| 五险一金有什么用| 前列腺按摩什么感觉| 胎儿双顶径偏大是什么原因| 淋巴结肿吃什么消炎药| 肝脑涂地是什么意思| 安全起见是什么意思| 乌贼是什么动物| 狗狗打疫苗前后要注意什么| 孕妇吃榴莲对胎儿有什么好处| 脱落细胞学检查是什么| 怀疑是什么意思| 汤沐邑是什么意思| 杠杠的是什么意思| 虾仁和什么包饺子好吃| 弄虚作假是什么生肖| 金庸原名叫什么| 更年期是什么意思| 薄情是什么意思| 陀螺是什么意思| 胸口闷痛挂什么科| 少田宅痣是什么意思| 天葬是什么| 96年属什么的| 康熙叫什么名字| 看痘痘挂什么科| 防晒霜和隔离霜有什么区别| 羊水少了对宝宝有什么影响| 三轮体空什么意思| 熊猫为什么被称为国宝| 肛门口瘙痒涂什么药膏| 什么叫御姐| 8月28号是什么星座| 火鸡面为什么这么贵| 五合是什么意思| 脂蛋白a是什么| 上火吃什么水果降火快| gloomy是什么意思| 解痉是什么意思| 眼睛老是肿着是什么原因造成的| dwi呈高信号什么意思| 拉稀拉水吃什么药管用| 什么是六道轮回| 麦露迪女鞋什么档次| 灰指甲什么症状| bioisland是什么牌子| 珊瑚红是什么颜色| 男士蛋皮痒用什么药| 风什么意思| 泰斗是什么意思| 喉咙疼是什么原因| 什么是人乳头瘤病毒| 承恩是什么意思| 生粉是什么粉| 珞字五行属什么| 多动症是什么原因造成的| 脉压是什么| 漪字五行属什么| 甘油三脂高是什么意思| 卡西欧手表属于什么档次| 摩羯后面是什么星座| 吃什么食物补气血| 青什么黄什么| 女人吃什么排湿气最快| 二月十七是什么星座| 眼睛酸疼是什么原因| 牛在五行中属什么| 白蛋白偏高是什么原因| crp高是什么意思| 肝郁气滞吃什么药好| 茯苓长什么样| 腰花是什么| 美仑美奂什么意思| 漂头发是什么意思| 老鼠爱吃什么食物| 成吉思汗什么意思| 什么是直女| 阴道炎是什么原因引起的| 樵是什么意思| pre什么意思| 个体差异是什么意思| 尿路感染吃什么药效果好| 中国黄金为什么比其它金店便宜| 七叶一枝花主治什么病| 有伤口吃什么消炎药| 志心皈命礼是什么意思| 合成立方氧化锆是什么| 国家栋梁指的是什么官| 难为你了是什么意思| 心率过快吃什么药好| 全身酸痛什么原因| 雷暴是什么意思| 喝什么茶养肝护肝最好| 心动过缓吃什么药| 为什么胃酸会分泌过多| 梦见蛇代表什么| 射手女跟什么星座最配| 耳朵发烫是什么征兆| 什么花草| 交友是什么意思| 3月30日是什么星座| 抽血能查出什么| 脚心疼痛是什么原因引起的| 猫驱虫药什么牌子好| 喝什么解酒| 清白是什么意思| 隔天是什么意思| 耳朵前面有痣代表什么| 岱字五行属什么| 普洱茶有什么功效与作用| 肺结节看什么科| 神经纤维是什么| 蜗牛的触角有什么作用| 肝部有阴影一般都是什么病| 3月14日是什么日子| 负担是什么意思| 胀气是什么症状| 心衰竭是什么症状| 吃饭时头晕是什么原因| 老子是什么时期的人| 低血压和低血糖有什么区别| 茄子与什么相克| 钝角是什么意思| 雪茄为什么不过肺| 低保是什么| 夏天容易出汗是什么原因| 过期茶叶有什么用途| pettm和pe有什么区别| 动脉血是什么颜色| 信阳毛尖属于什么茶| 趴着睡觉是什么原因| 心率过低有什么危害| 精卫填海是什么意思| 怦然心动什么意思| 什么文什么字| 3p 什么意思| 脾胃不好吃什么药| 尿骚味重是什么原因| 什么情况下要做宫腔镜| 施华洛世奇水晶是什么材质| 蜂蜡是什么| 哆啦a梦大结局是什么| 口腔异味是什么原因引起的| 9月份是什么星座的| 额头炎是什么症状| 金鱼吃什么| 粗茶淡饭下一句是什么| 黑曜石属于五行属什么| 胆汁反流性胃炎吃什么中成药| 狮子座是什么时候| 飞行模式有什么用| 李子有什么功效与作用| 舌苔发黄什么原因| 算命先生是什么生肖| 尿等待是什么原因| chloe是什么牌子| 山峦是什么意思| 果腹是什么意思| 针灸后需要注意什么| 女人十个簸箕是什么命| 孕期心情不好对胎儿有什么影响| 先兆性流产是什么症状| 梦到下雨是什么意思| 头发为什么会白| 令瓦念什么| 6.3是什么星座| 拉肚子是什么原因| pussy是什么意思| 成人高考是什么| 秀才相当于什么学历| 尿频尿急小腹胀痛吃什么药| 1884年属什么生肖| 鸡宝是什么| 接吻有什么好处| 腰椎生理曲度变直什么意思| 梦见打官司预示着什么| 空腹血糖高吃什么药| 草酸是什么| 什么的荷叶| 什么小吃最火爆最赚钱| app是什么缩写| 梦见自己吃面条是什么意思| 喝茶水对身体有什么好处| 黄瓜不能和什么食物一起吃| 花肠是母猪的什么部位| 薄凉是什么意思| 想做肠镜挂什么科| balmain什么档次| 618是什么日子| 金钱能买来什么但买不来什么| 抽电子烟有什么危害| 为什么会梦见前男友| 热爱是什么意思| 打胶原蛋白针有什么副作用吗| 梦见小孩是什么意思| 夏季吃什么菜| 补是什么偏旁| 蟹粉是什么| 眼干眼涩用什么眼药水| 脚底板痛什么原因| 灰色t恤配什么颜色裤子| 谍影重重4为什么换主角| 疑难杂症是什么意思| 过敏性鼻炎吃什么食物好| 百度

调查:台湾大学毕业生最想去台积电、最崇拜郭台铭

百度   年龄条件。

The phonological history of English includes various changes in the phonology of consonant clusters.

H-cluster reductions

edit

The H-cluster reductions are various consonant reductions that have occurred in the history of English, involving consonant clusters beginning with /h/ that have lost the /h/ (or become reduced to /h/) in some or all dialects.

Reductions of /hw/

edit

The cluster /hw/ (spelled ?wh? since Middle English) has been subject to two kinds of reduction:

  • Reduction to /h/ before rounded vowels (due to /hw/ being perceived as a /h/ with the labialization characteristic of that environment). This occurred with the word how in the Old English period, and with who, whom and whose in Middle English (the latter words having had an unrounded vowel in Old English).
  • Reduction to /w/, a development that has affected the speech of the great majority of English speakers, causing them to pronounce ?wh-? the same as ?w-? (sometimes called the wine–whine merger or glide cluster reduction). The distinction is maintained, however, in Scotland, most of Ireland, and some Southern American English.

Reduction of /hl/, /hr/ and /hn/

edit

The Old English consonant clusters /hl/, /hr/ and /hn/ were reduced to /l/, /r/, and /n/ in Middle English. For example, Old English hlāf, hring and hnutu become loaf, ring and nut in Modern English.

Reduction of /hj/

edit

In some dialects of English the cluster /hj/ is reduced to /j/,[1] leading to pronunciations like /ju?d?/ for huge and /?ju?m?n/ for human, and making hew, hue, and Hugh homophones of ewe, yew, and you. This is sometimes considered a type of glide cluster reduction, but it is much less widespread than wh-reduction, and is generally stigmatized where it is found. Aside from accents with general H-dropping, in the United States this reduction is mostly found in accents of Philadelphia and New York City; it also occurs in Cork accents of Irish English. In other dialects of English, hew and yew remain distinct; however, the cluster /hj/ of hew, human, etc. is often reduced from [?j] to just [?] (a voiceless palatal fricative).[2][3]

Y-cluster reductions

edit

Y-cluster reductions are reductions of clusters ending with the palatal approximant /j/, which is the sound of ?y? in yes, and is sometimes referred to as "yod", from the Hebrew letter yod(h), which has the sound [j]. Many such clusters arose in dialects in which the falling diphthong /?u/ (the product of the merger of several Middle English vowel sequences) became the rising diphthong /ju?/. (For more information, see Phonological history of English high back vowels.) They were thus often found before the vowel /u?/, as in cube /kju?b/ – which was in some cases modified to /??/ or /?/ before (historical) /r/, as in cure, or weakened to /?/ or /?/ as in argument. They also occurred in words ending in -ion and -ious, such as nation and precious.

This change from /?u/ to /ju?/, which had occurred in London by the end of the 17th century, did not take place in all dialects. A few dialects, notably in Wales, as well as in some parts of northern England, New England, and the American South, still retain a (falling) /?u/ diphthong where standard English has /ju?/ – these dialects therefore lack the clusters with /j/ and have not been subject to the reductions described here.[4]

The diphthongs /ju?/ or /???/ are most commonly indicated by the spellings ?eu?, ?ew?, ?uCV? (where C is any consonant and V is any vowel), ?ue? and ?ui?, as in feud, few, mute, cue and suit, while the historical monophthong /u?/ is commonly indicated by the spellings ?oo? and ?ou?, as in moon and soup.

Yod-dropping

edit

Yod-dropping is the elision of the /j/ from certain syllable-initial clusters of the type described above. Particular cases of yod-dropping may affect all or some of the dialects that have the relevant clusters.

The change of [?] to [j] in these positions (as described above) produced some clusters which would have been difficult or impossible to pronounce, which led to what John Wells calls "early yod dropping" in which the [j] was elided in the following environments:[5]

  • After /?, t?, d?/, for example chute /?u?t/, chew /t?u?/, juice /d?u?s/
  • After /j/, for example yew /ju?/ (compare [j???] in some conservative dialects)
  • After /r/, for example rude /ru?d/
  • After stop+/l/ clusters, for example blue /blu?/

The previously mentioned accents that did not have the [?][j] change were not subject to this process. Thus, for example, in much Welsh English pairs like chews/choose, yew/you and threw/through remain distinct: the first member of each pair has the diphthong /???/, while the second member has /u?/:[6]

  • chews /t????z/, choose /t?u?z/
  • yew /j???/, you /ju?/
  • threw /θr???/, through /θru?/

Conversely, an initial /j/ does not appear in Welsh English before /i?/ in words such as yeast and yield.[7]

Many varieties of English have extended yod-dropping to the following environments if the /j/ is in the same syllable as the preceding consonant:

  • After /s/, for example suit /su?t/
  • After /l/, for example lute /?lu?t/
  • After /z/, for example Zeus /?zu?s/
  • After /θ/, for example enthuse /?n?θu?z/

Yod-dropping in the above environments used to be considered nonstandard in England but now also occurs by educated RP-speakers.[8] (The /j/ after /s/ is not normally dropped in RP in medial positions, however: compare pursuit /p??sju?t/.) In General American, yod-dropping is found not only in the above environments but also after /t/, /d/ and /n/ (for example, tune /?tu?n/, dew /?du?/, and new /?nu?/).

The lack of yod-dropping in those contexts has occasionally been held to be a shibboleth distinguishing Canadians from Americans. However, in a survey conducted in the Golden Horseshoe area of Southern Ontario in 1994, over 80% of respondents under the age of 40 pronounced student and news without yod.[9]

 
The areas marked in pink show where in the United States a distinction between /???/ in dew and /u/ in do may be made.[10]

General American thus undergoes yod-dropping after all alveolar consonants. A few accents of American English, such as working-class Southern American English, however, preserve the distinction in pairs like do/dew because, like in the Welsh English dialects discussed above, they retain a diphthong /???/ in words in which RP has /ju?/: /lut~l???t/, /du~d???/, etc.[10]

However, in words like annual, menu, volume, Matthew, continue, etc., with a syllable break before the /j/, there is no yod-dropping. The same applies accordingly to British and other accents; the yod is often dropped after initial /l/, for example, but it is not dropped in words like volume or value. (British speakers omit the /j/ in figure, but most Americans retain it.)

Additionally, there is no /j/ in British pronunciations of coupon and Pulitzer, /?ku?p?n/ and /?p?l?ts?/ respectively, but many American speakers keep the yod, realizing them as /?kju?p?n/ and /?pju?l?ts?r/, although Pulitzer with the pew sound is widely incorrect.[11][12]

In New Zealand and to some extent Australian English, debut is mainly pronounced without the yod as /?d?eb??/.[13]

Yod-dropping after /t/, /d/, and /n/ was also a traditional feature of Cockney speech, which continues to be the case after /n/, but now, after /t/ and /d/, yod-coalescence is now more common.[14]

Some East Anglian accents such as Norfolk dialect extend yod-dropping not only to the position after /t/, /d/ or /n/ but also to the position after nonalveolar consonants as well: pairs like beauty/booty, mute/moot, cute/coot can then be homophonous.[15] A well-known series of British television advertisements beginning in the 1980s featured Bernard Matthews, who was from Norfolk and described his turkeys as "bootiful" (for beautiful). Such accents pronounce a /j/ in words like "use", "unit", etc. only if there is no consonant before the /j/.

Homophonous pairs after j, r, ?, and t?
/???/ /u?/ IPA Notes
brewed brood ?bru?d
brume broom ?bru?m
chews choose ?t?u?z
chute shoot ??u?t
drupe droop ?dru?p
rheum room ?ru?m
rude rood ?ru?d
rue roo ?ru?
ruse roos ?ru?z
threw through ?θru?
yew you ?ju?
yule you'll ?ju?l
Homophonous pairs after l and s
/???/ /u?/ IPA Notes
Blume bloom ?blu?m
glume gloom ?glu?m
Lewis Louis ?lu??s
lieu loo ?lu?
lieu Lou ?lu?
Luke look ?lu?k With foot–goose merger.
lune loon ?lu?n
lute loot ?lu?t
slew slough ?slu?
slue slough ?slu?
sue Sioux ?su?
suit soot ?su?t With foot–goose merger.
Homophonous pairs after d, n, and t
/???/ /u?/ IPA Notes
adieu ado ??du?
dew do ?du?
Dewar doer ?du??r
due do ?du?
dune Doon ?du?n
knew nu ?nu?
new nu ?nu?
tune toon ?tu?n
Homophonous pairs after other consonants
/???/ /u?/ IPA Notes
beaut boot ?bu?t
beauty booty ?bu?ti
butte boot ?bu?t
cue coo ?ku?
cute coot ?ku?t
feud food ?fu?d
few foo ?fu?
fuel fool ?fu?l With vile–vial merger.
hew who ?hu?
hews who's ?hu?z
hews whose ?hu?z
hue who ?hu?
hues who's ?hu?z
hues whose ?hu?z
Hugh who ?hu?
Hughes who's ?hu?z
Hughes whose ?hu?z
Kew coo ?ku?
kyu coo ?ku?
mew moo ?mu?
mew moue ?mu?
mewed mood ?mu?d
muse moos ?mu?z
muse moues ?mu?z
mute moot ?mu?t
pew poo ?pu?
pule pool ?pu?l
pure poor ?p??(r)
Q; cue coo ?ku?
que coo ?ku?
queue coo ?ku?

Yod-coalescence

edit

Yod-coalescence is a process that fuses the clusters /dj, tj, sj, zj/ into the sibilants [d?, t?, ?, ?] respectively (for the meanings of those symbols, see English phonology). The first two are examples of affrication.

Unlike yod-dropping, yod-coalescence frequently occurs with clusters that would be considered to span a syllable boundary and so commonly occurs before unstressed syllables. For example, in educate, the /dj/ cluster would not usually be subject to yod-dropping in General American, as the /d/ is assigned to the previous syllable, but it commonly coalesces to [d?]. Here are a few examples of yod-coalescence universal in all English dialects:

  • /tj/ [t?] in most words ending -ture, such as nature [?ne?t???]
  • /dj/ [d?] in soldier [?so?ld???]
  • /sj/ [?] in words ending with -ssure such as pressure [?p?????] (also in words ending with consonant+sure, consonant+sion, -tion)
  • /zj/ [?] in words ending vowel+sure such as measure [?m????] (also vowel+sion, r+sion)

In some other words, the coalesced pronunciation is common in English dialects around the world, but an older non-coalesced form still exists among some speakers of standard British English:

  • educate [??d??ke?t] (also in standard RP: [??dj?ke?t])
  • azure [?????] (also in RP [??zj??])
  • issue [???u?] (also in RP [??sju?]), the intermediate form [???ju?] being also common

Coalescence can even occur across word boundaries, as in the colloquial "gotcha" /?ɡ?t??/ (for got you /?ɡ?tju/) and "whatcha" /?w?t??/ (for what're you /?w?t?rj?/).

In certain English accents, yod-coalescence also occurs in stressed syllables, as in tune and dune. That occurs in Australian, Cockney, Estuary English, Zimbabwean English, some speakers of Hiberno-English, Newfoundland English, South African English, and to a certain extent[16] in New Zealand English, RP,[17] many speakers in Scottish English, and even some varieties of English in Asia, like Philippine English (many speakers because of the influence by the phonology of their mother languages). That results in pronunciations such as the following:

  • dew/due [d?u?] (RP: [dju?])
  • tune [t?u?n] (RP: [tju?n])

In certain varieties such as Australian, Ugandan, and some RP,[17] stressed [sj, zj] can also coalesce:

  • resume [????u?m] (RP: [???zju?m])
  • assume [???u?m] (RP: [??sju?m])

That can lead to additional homophony; for instance, dew and due come to be pronounced the same as Jew.

Yod-coalescence has traditionally been resisted in Received Pronunciation. It has certainly become established in words of the first group listed above (nature, soldier, pressure etc.), but it is not yet universal in those of the second group (educate etc.), and it does not generally occur in those of the third group (dew, tune etc.).[18]

Homophonous pairs
/?u/ /ju?/ IPA Notes
deuce juice ?d?u?s
dew Jew ?d?u?
dewed Jude ?d?u?d
dual jewel ?d?u??l
due Jew ?d?u?
duel jewel ?d?u??l
duke juke ?d?u?k
duly Julie ?d?u?li
dune June ?d?u?n
duty Judy ?d?u??i With intervocalic alveolar flapping.
sue shoe ??u?
sue shoo ??u?
suit chute ??u?t
suit shoot ??u?t
'tude chewed ?t?u?d

See also

Other initial cluster reductions

edit

Reduction of /wr/ and /wl/

edit

Old and Middle English had an initial /wr/ cluster (note that /r/ does not denote [?] here), hence the spelling of words like write and wrong. This was reduced to just /r/, apparently during the 17th century. An intermediate stage may have been an [r] with lip rounding.[19]

As a result of this reduction, pairs of words like rap and wrap, rite and write, etc. are homophones in practically all varieties of Modern English. They remain distinct in the Doric dialect of Scots, where the wr- cluster is pronounced /vr/. Alexander John Ellis reported distinctions between wr and r in Cumbria and in several varieties of Scots in the nineteenth century.[20]

Old English also had a cluster /wl/, which reduced to /l/ during Middle English. For example, the word lisp derives from Old English wlisp(ian).

Reduction of /kn/

edit

Middle English initial /kn/ is reduced in modern English to /n/, making pairs like knot/not and knight/night homophones.

The /kn/ cluster was spelled cn- in Old English; this changed to kn- in Middle English, and this spelling survives in Modern English, despite the loss of the /k/ sound. Cognates in other Germanic languages usually still sound the initial /k/. For example, the Old English ancestor of knee was cnēo, pronounced /kne?o?/, and the cognate word in Modern German is Knie, pronounced /kni?/.

Most dialects of English reduced the initial cluster /kn/ to /n/ relatively recently; the change seems to have taken place in educated English during the 17th century.[21] Several German-language grammars of English from the late 17th and early 18th centuries transcribed English kn- as tn-, dn-, implying that a stage of assimilation (or perhaps debuccalization to /?n/) preceded that of complete reduction.[22]

The cluster is preserved in some Scots dialects,[23] and Alexander John Ellis recorded it in parts of the Northern English counties of Cumbria and Northumberland in the late nineteenth century.[24]

Reduction of /ɡn/

edit

The Middle English initial cluster /ɡn/ is reduced to /n/ in Modern English. Like the reduction of /kn/, this seems to have taken place during the seventeenth century.[25] The change affected words like gnat, gnostic, gnome, etc., the spelling with gn- being retained despite the loss of the /ɡ/ sound. The cluster is preserved in some Scots dialects.[23]

The song The Gnu jokes about this silent g and other silent letters in English. In fact the g in gnu may always have been silent in English, since this loanword did not enter the language until the late 18th century.[26] The trumpeter Kenny Wheeler wrote a composition titled Gnu High, a pun on "new high".

S-cluster reductions

edit

In some types of Caribbean English, the initial clusters /sp/, /st/, and /sk/ are reduced by the loss of /s/. The following stop is then subject to regular aspiration (or devoicing of the following approximant) in its new word-initial environment. Some examples of such pronunciations are:

spit 'pit [?sp?t] [?p??t]
stomach 'tomach [?st?m?k] [?t??m?k]
spend 'pen [?sp?nd] [?p??n] (also affected by final cluster reduction)
squeeze 'queeze [?skwi?z] [?k?w?i?z]

According to Wells, these reductions occur only in the broadest creole.[27]

Final cluster reductions

edit

NG-coalescence

edit

NG-coalescence is a historical sound change by which the final cluster /nɡ/, pronounced [?ɡ] (the /n/ being realized as a velar nasal by assimilation with the velar /ɡ/), came to be pronounced as just [?] – that is, the final [ɡ] was dropped, but the velar quality of the nasal remained. The change took place in educated London speech around the end of the 16th century, and explains why there is no [ɡ] sound at the end of words like fang, sing, wrong and tongue in the standard varieties of Modern English.[28]

The change applies not only at the end of a word, but generally at the end of a morpheme. If a word ending in -ng is followed by a suffix or is compounded with another word, the [?] pronunciation normally remains. For example, in the words fangs, sings, singing, singer, wronged, wrongly, hangman, there is no [ɡ] sound. An exception is the comparative and superlative forms of adjectives: in the words longer/longest, stronger/strongest, younger/youngest, the [ɡ] is pronounced in most accents. The pronunciation with [ɡ] is thus possible only before a vowel; before a consonant, the only possibility is a bare [?].

In other cases (when it is not morpheme-final), word-internal -ng- does not show the effects of coalescence, and the pronunciation [?ɡ] is retained, as in finger and angle. This means that the words finger and singer do not rhyme in most modern varieties of English, although they did in Middle English. The process of NG-coalescence might therefore be referred to as the singer–finger split.

 
Pronunciation of ?ng? in the word tongue in various regional dialects of England

Some accents, however, do not show the full effects of NG-coalescence as described above. In these accents, sing may be found with [?ɡ], and singer may rhyme with finger.[29] This is particularly associated with English English accents in areas such as Lancashire, the West Midlands and Derbyshire, and is also present in north-east varieties of Welsh English. This includes the cities of Birmingham (see Brummie), Manchester (see Manchester dialect), Liverpool (see Scouse), Sheffield and Stoke-on-Trent (see Potteries dialect). This also occurs in a small area of Kent. As this occurs around the mining area of Kent, it might be a result of large-scale migration by miners from other more northerly coalfields to Kent in the 1920s.

It is also associated with some American English accents in the New York City area.[30]

On the other hand, in some accents of the west of Scotland and Ulster, NG-coalescence is extended to morpheme-internal position, so that finger is pronounced /?f???r/ (cf. Dutch vinger /?v???r/), thus rhyming with singer (although the [ɡ] is not dropped before a stressed syllable, as in engage).

It is because of NG-coalescence that /?/ is now normally regarded one of the phonemes of standard English. In Middle English, the [?] can be regarded as an allophone of /n/, occurring before velar consonants, but in Modern English, in view of minimal pairs such as pan–pang and sin–sing, that analysis no longer appears to hold. Nevertheless, some linguists[who?] (particularly generativists) do regard a word like sing as being underlyingly /s?nɡ/, positing a rule that deletes [ɡ] after a nasal before a morpheme boundary, after the nasal has undergone assimilation. A problem with this view is that there are a few words in which [?] is followed neither by a velar nor a morpheme boundary (such as gingham, dinghy, orangutan and Singapore, for those speakers who pronounce them without [ɡ]), and some in which the [ɡ] is not deleted before a morpheme boundary (such as longer, stronger, younger noted above). In the case of longer, a minimal pair occurs for some speakers between /l??ɡ?(r)/ (comparative form of the adjective long) and /l???(r)/ ("someone who longs"; agent noun of the verb long).

The above-mentioned accents which lack NG-coalescence may more easily be analyzed as lacking a phoneme /?/. The same may apply to those where NG-coalescence is extended to morpheme-internal position, since here a more consistent [ɡ]-deletion rule can be formulated.[31]

G-dropping

edit

G-dropping is a popular name for the feature of speech whereby /n/ is used in place of the standard /?/ in weak syllables. This applies especially to the -ing ending of verbs, but also in other words such as morning, nothing, ceiling, Buckingham, etc. G-dropping speakers may pronounce this syllable as [?n] or [?n] (reducing to a syllabic [n] in some cases), while non-G-dropping speakers have /??/ (/??/ with the weak vowel merger) or /i?/.[32]

Relative to the great majority of modern dialects, which have NG-coalescence, G-dropping does not involve the dropping of any sound, simply the replacement of the velar nasal with the alveolar nasal. The name derives from the apparent orthographic consequence of replacing the sound written ?ng? with that normally written ?n?. The spelling -in' is sometimes used to indicate that a speaker uses the G-dropping pronunciation, as in makin' for making.

The pronunciation with /n/ rather than /?/ is a long-established one. Old English verbs had a present participle in -ende and a verbal noun (gerund) form in -ing(e). These merged into a single form, written -ing, but not necessarily spoken as such – the /n/ pronunciation may be inherited from the former distinct present participle form. The /n/ variant appears to have been fashionable generally during the 18th century, with the alternative /??/ being adopted in educated speech around the 1820s, possibly as a spelling pronunciation.[33]

Today, G-dropping is a feature of colloquial and non-standard speech of all regions, including stereotypically of Cockney, Southern American English and African American Vernacular English. Its use is highly correlated with the socioeconomic class of the speaker, with speakers of lower classes using /n/ with greater frequency. It has also been found to be more common among men than women, and less common in more formal styles of speech.[34]

The fact that the /n/ pronunciation was formerly associated with certain upper-class speech is reflected in the phrase huntin’, shootin’ and fishin’ (used in referring to country gentry who frequently engaged in such field sports). Further evidence that this pronunciation was once standard comes from old rhymes, as in this couplet from John Gay's 1732 pastoral Acis and Galatea, set to music by Handel:

Shepherd, what art thou pursuing,
Heedless running to thy ruin?

which was presumably pronounced "shepherd, what art thou pursuin', heedless runnin' to thy ruin", although this would sound very odd in an opera today. Similarly, in the poetry of Jonathan Swift (1667–1745), -ing forms consistently rhyme with words ending in /?n/, as in this verse of A Ballad on the Game of Traffic, where "lining" rhymes with "fine in":

But Weston has a new-cast gown
On Sundays to be fine in,
And, if she can but win a crown,
'Twill just new dye the lining.

Reduction of /mb/ and /mn/

edit

In later Middle English, the final cluster /mb/ was reduced to just /m/ (the plum-plumb merger). This affects words such as lamb and plumb, as well as derived forms with suffixes, such as lambs, lambing, plumbed, plumber.

By analogy with words like these, certain other words ending in /m/, which had no historical /b/ sound, had a silent letter ?b? added to their spelling by way of hypercorrection. Such words include limb and crumb.[35]

Where the final cluster /mn/ occurred, this was reduced to /m/ (the him-hymn merger), as in column, autumn, damn, solemn. (Compare French automne, where the cluster has been reduced to /n/.) Both sounds are nonetheless still pronounced before vowels in certain derivatives, such as columnar, autumnal, damnation, solemnity.

Generalized final cluster reduction

edit

General reduction of final consonant clusters occurs in African American Vernacular English and Caribbean English. It also appears in the Local Dublin English.[36] The new final consonant may be slightly lengthened as an effect.

Examples are:

test tes' [t?st] [t?s(?)]
desk des' [d?sk] [d?s(?)]
hand han' [h?nd] [h?n(?)]
send sen' [s?nd] [s?n(?)]
left lef' [l?ft] [l?f(?)]
wasp was' [w?sp] [w?s(?)]

The plurals of test and desk may become tesses and desses by the same rule that gives plural messes from singular mess.[37][38][39][40]

Medial cluster reductions

edit

When a consonant cluster ending in a stop is followed by another consonant or cluster in the next syllable, the final stop in the first syllable is often elided. This may happen within words or across word boundaries. Examples of stops that will often be elided in this way include the [t] in postman and the [d] in cold cuts or band saw.[41]

Historically, similar reductions have taken place before syllabic consonants in certain words, leading to the silent ?t? in words like castle and listen. This change took place around the 17th century. In the word often, the [t] sound later came to be re-inserted by some speakers as a spelling pronunciation.[42]

An earlier reduction that took place in early Middle English was the change of /ts/ to /s/ (the sent-cent merger). This led to the modern sound of soft ?c?.

Consonant insertions

edit

Prince–prints merger

edit

For many speakers, an epenthetic [t] is inserted in the final cluster /ns/, making it identical or very similar to the cluster /nts/. For example, the words prince and prints have come to be homophones or nearly so.

The epenthesis is a natural consequence of the transition from the nasal [n] to the fricative [s]; if the raising of the soft palate (which converts a nasal to an oral sound) is completed before the release of the tongue tip (which enables a fricative sound), an intervening stop [t] naturally results.[43] The merger of /ns/ and /nts/ is not necessarily complete, however; the duration of the epenthetic [t] in /ns/ has been found to be often shorter (and the [n] longer) than in the underlying cluster /nts/.[44] Some speakers preserve a clearer distinction, with prince having [ns], and prints having [nts] or [n?s]. The epenthesis does not occur between syllables, in words like consider.[45]

Other insertions

edit

The merger of /nz/ and /ndz/ is also possible, making bans and pens sound like bands and pends. However, this is less common than the merger of /ns/ and /nts/ described above, and in rapid speech may involve the elision of the /d/ from /ndz/ rather than epenthesis in /nz/.[46]

Epenthesis of a stop between a nasal and a fricative can also occur in other environments, for example:

  • /n?/ may become /nt?/ (so pinscher is often pronounced like pincher)
  • /ms/ may become /mps/ (so Samson becomes "Sampson", hamster becomes "hampster")
  • /?s/ may become /?ks/ (so Kingston becomes "kinkston")[46]

Epenthesis may also happen in the cluster /ls/, which then becomes /lts/, so else rhymes with belts.

An epenthetic [p] often intervenes in the cluster /mt/ in the word dreamt, making it rhyme with attempt.

Some originally epenthetic consonants have become part of the established pronunciation of words. This applies, for instance, to the /b/ in words like thimble, grumble and scramble.[35]

For the insertion of glottal stops before certain consonants, see Glottalization below.

Homophonous pairs
fricative affricate IPA Notes
Aaron's errands ??r?n(d)z With Mary-marry-merry merger.
-ance -ants -?n(t)s
ANSI antsy ??n(t)si
bans bands ?b?n(d)z
Ben's bends ?b?n(d)z
bines binds ?ba?n(d)z
brans brands ?br?n(d)z
bunce bunts ?b?n(t)s
Bynes binds ?ba?n(d)z
chance chants ?t??n(t)s, ?t?ɑ?n(t)s
dense dents ?d?n(t)s
dense dints ?d?n(t)s With pen-pin merger.
-ence -ents -?n(t)s
Erin's errands ??r?n(d)z With weak vowel merger.
fines finds ?fa?n(d)z
fens fends ?f?n(d)z
Finns fends ?f?n(d)z With pen-pin merger.
fins fends ?f?n(d)z With pen-pin merger.
glans glands ?ɡl?n(d)z
Hans hands ?h?n(d)z Hans may also be pronounced /?hɑ?nz/ or /?hɑ?ns/.
Heinz hinds ?ha?n(d)z Heinz may also be pronounced /?ha?nts/.
hence hints ?h?n(t)s With pen-pin merger.
Hines hinds ?ha?n(d)z
inns ends ??n(d)z With pen-pin merger.
ins ends ??n(d)z With pen-pin merger.
intense intents ?n?t?n(t)s
Kines kinds ?ka?n(d)z
LANs lands ?l?n(d)z
lens lends ?l?n(d)z
men's mends ?m?n(d)z
mince mints ?m?n(t)s
mines minds ?ma?n(d)z
N's; ens ends ??n(d)z
patience patients ?pe???n(t)s
pawns ponds ?pɑn(d)z With cot-caught merger.
pens pends ?p?n(d)z
pins pends ?p?n(d)z With pen-pin merger.
ponce ponts ?pɑn(t)s
pons ponds ?pɑn(d)z
presence presents ?pr?z?n(t)s
prince prints ?pr?n(t)s
rinse rents ?r?n(t)s With pen-pin merger.
sans sands ?s?n(d)z
sense cents ?s?n(t)s
sense scents ?s?n(t)s
since cents ?s?n(t)s With pen-pin merger.
since scents ?s?n(t)s With pen-pin merger.
spins spends ?sp?n(d)z With pen-pin merger.
Stan's stands ?st?n(d)z
tens tends ?t?n(d)z
tense tents ?t?n(t)s
tense tints ?t?n(t)s With pen-pin merger.
tins tends ?t?n(d)z With pen-pin merger.
Vince vents ?v?n(t)s With pen-pin merger.
wans wands ?wɑn(d)z
wens wends ?w?n(d)z
wens winds (n.) ?w?n(d)z With pen-pin merger.
wince Wentz ?w?n(t)s With pen-pin merger.
whence Wentz ?w?n(t)s With wine-whine merger.
whines winds (v.) ?wa?n(d)z With wine-whine merger.
wines winds (v.) ?wa?n(d)z
wins wends ?w?n(d)z With pen-pin merger.
wins winds (n.) ?w?n(d)z
wyns, wynns wends ?w?n(d)z With pen-pin merger.
wyns, wynns winds (n.) ?w?n(d)z

Alterations of clusters

edit

Assimilation

edit

In English as in other languages, assimilation of adjacent consonants is common, particularly of a nasal with a following consonant. This can occur within or between words. For example, the /n/ in encase is often pronounced [?] (becoming a velar nasal by way of assimilation with the following velar stop /k/), and the /n/ in ten men likely becomes [m], assimilating with the following bilabial nasal /m/. Other cases of assimilation also occur, such as pronunciation of the /d/ in bad boy as [b]. Voicing assimilation determines the sound of the endings -s (as in plurals, possessives and verb forms) and -ed (in verb forms): these are voiced ([z], [d]) following a voiced consonant (or vowel), but voiceless ([s], [t]) after a voiceless consonant, as in gets, knocked.[47]

Glottalization

edit

While there are many accents (such as Cockney) in which syllable-final /t/ is frequently glottalized (realized as a glottal stop, [?]) regardless of what follows it, the glottaling of /t/ in clusters is a feature even of standard accents, such as RP. There, [?] may be heard for /t/ in such words and phrases as quite good, quite nice, nights. More precisely, it occurs in RP when /t/ appears in the syllable coda, is preceded by a vowel, liquid or nasal, and it is followed by another consonant except (normally) a liquid or semivowel in the same word, as in mattress.[48]

Another possibility is pre-glottalization (or glottal reinforcement), where a glottal stop is inserted before a syllable-final stop, rather than replacing it. That can happen before /p/, /t/ and /k/ or also before the affricate /t?/. It can occur in RP in the same environments as those mentioned above, without the final restriction so a glottal stop may appear before the /t/, as in mattress. It can also occur before a pause as in quite! spoken alone but not in quite easy. In the case of /t?/, pre-glottalization is common even before a vowel, as in teacher.[49]

According to Wells, this pre-glottalization originated in the 20th century (at least, it was not recorded until then). Glottalization of /t/ spread rapidly during the 20th century.[48]

S-cluster metathesis

edit

Final consonant clusters starting with /s/ sometimes undergo metathesis, meaning that the order of the consonants is switched. For example, the word ask may be pronounced like "ax", with the /k/ and the /s/ switched.

This example has a long history: the Old English verb áscian also appeared as acsian, and both forms continued into Middle English, the latter, metathesizing to "ask". The form axe appears in Chaucer: "I axe, why the fyfte man Was nought housband to the Samaritan?" (Wife of Bath's Prologue, 1386), and was considered acceptable in literary English until about 1600.[50][failed verification] It persists in some dialects of rural England as well as in Ulster Scots[51] as /?aks/, and in Jamaican English as /?a?ks/, from where it has entered London English as /?ɑ?ks/.

S-cluster metathesis has been observed in some forms of African American Vernacular English, although it is not universal, one of the most stigmatized features of AAVE and often commented on by teachers.[37][failed verification] Examples of possible AAVE pronunciations include:

ask /??ks/
grasp /?ɡr?ps/
wasp /?wɑps/
gasp /?ɡ?ps/

Merger of /str/ and /skr/

edit

For some speakers of African American Vernacular English, the consonant cluster /str/ is pronounced as /skr/. For example, the word street may be pronounced as /skrit/.[52]

The form has been found to occur in Gullah and in the speech of some young African Americans born in the Southern United States. It is reported to be a highly stigmatized feature, with children who use it often being referred to speech pathologists.[53]

Yod-rhotacization

edit

Yod-rhotacization is a process that occurs for some Memphis AAVE[37] speakers, where /j/ is rhotacized to [r] in consonant clusters, causing pronunciations like:

beautiful [?bru??fl?]
cute [krut]
music [?mruz?k]

Compare yod-dropping and yod-coalescence, described above (and also the coil–curl merger, which features the reverse process, /r//j/).

See also

edit

References

edit
  1. ^ "Archived copy". Archived from the original on 2025-08-05. Retrieved 2025-08-05.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: archived copy as title (link)
  2. ^ Gimson, A. C. (1980). An Introduction to the Pronunciation of English (3rd ed.). London: Edward Arnold Publishers. ISBN 0-7131-6287-2.
  3. ^ Ladefoged, Peter (2001). A Course in Phonetics (4th ed.). Fort Worth, Texas: Harcourt College Publishers. ISBN 0-15-507319-2.
  4. ^ Wells, John C. (1982). Accents of English. Vol. 1. Cambridge University Press. p. 207. ISBN 0-521-22919-7.
  5. ^ Wells (1982), p. 207.
  6. ^ Wells (1982), p. 385.
  7. ^ Mees, Inger M.; Collins, Beverley (1999). "Cardiff: A Real-time Study of Glottalisation". In Foulkes, Paul; Docherty, Gerard (eds.). Urban Voices. Arnold. p. 192. ISBN 0-340-70608-2.
  8. ^ Wells (1982), p. 206.
  9. ^ "Changes in Progress in Canadian English: Yod-dropping". CHASS.UToronto.ca. University of Toronto. Archived from the original on February 29, 2008. Retrieved March 30, 2010. Excerpts from: Chambers, J. K. (1998). "Social embedding of changes in progress". Journal of English Linguistics. 26: 5–36. doi:10.1177/007542429802600102. S2CID 144942447.
  10. ^ a b Labov, William; Ash, Sharon; Boberg, Charles (2006). The Atlas of North American English. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. ISBN 3-11-016746-8.
  11. ^ Duryee, Tricia (6 November 2011). "A Nation Divided on How to Say the Word "Coupon"". All Things D. Dow Jones & Company Inc.
  12. ^ "FAQ". The Pulitzer Prizes. Columbia University. 24. How is 'Pulitzer' pronounced? The correct pronunciation is 'PULL it sir.'
  13. ^ L. J. Bauer; P. Warren (2008). "New Zealand English: phonology". In Burridge, Kate; Kortmann, Bernd (eds.). Varieties of English 3: The Pacific and Australasia. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter. p. 60. ISBN 9783110208412.
  14. ^ Wells (1982), p. 330.
  15. ^ Wells (1982), p. 338.
  16. ^ Bauer, L.; Warren, P. (2005). "New Zealand English: Phonology". In Schneider, E. W. (ed.). A Handbook of Varieties of English: Phonology. Vol. 1. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. ISBN 9783110175325.
  17. ^ a b Why some say CHUBE and some say TOOB, 8 November 2022, retrieved 2025-08-05
  18. ^ Wells (1982), p. 247.
  19. ^ Jespersen, O., A Modern English Grammar on Historical Principles, vol. 1, 12.81-82.
  20. ^ "Ellis Atlas survival of distinction between wr- and r-". www.lel.ed.ac.uk. Retrieved 2025-08-05.
  21. ^ Jespersen, O., A Modern English Grammar on Historical Principles, vol. 1, 12.71.
  22. ^ Vietor, Wilhelm: Elemente der Phonetik und Orthoepie des Deutschen, Englischen und Franz?sischen, 2nd ed., Heilbronn, 1887, p. 171
  23. ^ a b "Wir Ain Leed - Mid Northern Scots". Scots Online. Retrieved 21 March 2020.
  24. ^ "Ellis Atlas survival of distinction between kn- and n-". www.lel.ed.ac.uk. Retrieved 2025-08-05.
  25. ^ Jespersen, O., A Modern English Grammar on Historical Principles, vol. 1, 12.72.
  26. ^ The first recorded use of the word gnu in English dates back to 1777, according to Merriam-Webster's dictionary.
  27. ^ Wells (1982), p. 567.
  28. ^ Wells (1982), p. 188.
  29. ^ Bailey, George (15 December 2020). "Insertion and deletion in Northern English (ng): Interacting innovations in the life cycle of phonological processes". Journal of Linguistics. 57 (3). Cambridge University Press: 465–497. doi:10.1017/S0022226720000365.
  30. ^ Wells (1982), pp. 189, 366.
  31. ^ Wells (1982), pp. 60–64.
  32. ^ Wells (1982), p. 262.
  33. ^ Wyld, H.C., A History of Modern Colloquial English, Blackwell 1936, cited in Wells (1982), p. 262.
  34. ^ Wells (1982), pp. 17, 19, 26.
  35. ^ a b Liberman, Anatoly (21 October 2009). "The Oddest and Dumbest English Spellings, Part 15, With a Note on Words and Things". OUP. Retrieved 28 January 2015.
  36. ^ * Hickey, Raymond (2007). Irish English: History and Present Day Forms. Cambridge University Press. p. 353-354. ISBN 9781139465847.
  37. ^ a b c "Phonological Features of African American Vernacular English (AAVE)". www.rehabmed.ualberta.ca. March 17, 2001.
  38. ^ HLW: Word Forms: Processes: English Accents
  39. ^ List of AAVE features contrasting with MUSE Archived 2025-08-05 at the Wayback Machine
  40. ^ Rickford, John R. (December 1996). "Ebonics Notes and Discussion". Stanford University. Archived from the original on 3 April 2025.
  41. ^ Denham, K., Lobeck, A., Linguistics for Everyone: An Introduction, Cengage Learning 2012, p. 162.
  42. ^ Algeo, J., Butcher, C. The Origins and Development of the English Language, Cengage Learning 2013, p. 49.
  43. ^ Wells (1982), p. 95.
  44. ^ Yu, A.C.L., in The Blackwell Companion to Phonology, Wiley 2011, p. 1906.
  45. ^ Wells, J.C., "Some day my prints will come", John Wells's Phonetic Blog, 25 August 2010.
  46. ^ a b Alan Cruttenden, Gimson's Pronunciation of English, Routledge 2013, p. 99.
  47. ^ Nathan, G.S., Phonology: A Cognitive Grammar Introduction, John Benjamins Publishing 2008, pp. 77–78.
  48. ^ a b Wells (1982), p. 261.
  49. ^ Wells (1982), p. 260.
  50. ^ Online Etymology Dictionary - Ask
  51. ^ Kperogi, Farooq A. (2015). Glocal English: The Changing Face and Forms of Nigerian English in a Global World. Peter Lang. p. 208. ISBN 978-1-4331-2926-1.
  52. ^ Green, Lisa J. (2002). African American English : a linguistic introduction (1. publ., 4. print. ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p. 122. ISBN 978-0521891387.
  53. ^ Dandy, E.B., Black Communications: Breaking Down the Barriers, African American Images, 1991, p. 44.
mastercard是什么意思 95年的猪是什么命 云南白药里的保险子有什么作用 子宫内膜息肉样增生是什么意思 淼怎么读什么意思
头皮屑大块是什么原因 骨痂形成是什么意思 兔子能吃什么水果 淋巴细胞比率偏高是什么原因 手淫对身体有什么伤害
身上长扁平疣是什么原因造成的 mz是什么意思 毋庸置疑什么意思 梦见缝被子是什么意思 为什么家里会有蚂蚁
紫外线过敏用什么药 未时属什么生肖 s标志的运动鞋是什么牌子 颈部疼痛挂什么科 脂肪肝吃什么药
灰色裤子配什么上衣hcv8jop1ns4r.cn 雌蕊由什么组成hcv9jop7ns2r.cn 猪八戒原名叫什么hcv8jop0ns9r.cn 入户口需要什么资料hcv8jop9ns0r.cn 画蛇添足告诉我们什么道理hcv8jop3ns2r.cn
空调为什么要加氟hcv7jop4ns7r.cn 喝咖啡有什么好处和坏处jasonfriends.com 令香是什么意思hcv7jop5ns1r.cn 走之底的字与什么有关hcv8jop6ns1r.cn 老卵上海话什么意思hcv8jop6ns7r.cn
什么原因导致子宫内膜息肉ff14chat.com 牙龈为什么会肿痛hcv9jop0ns3r.cn 弹性工作是什么意思hcv8jop2ns4r.cn 怀孕初期什么症状bjcbxg.com 清明节有什么习俗hcv8jop1ns2r.cn
松茸是什么hcv9jop2ns6r.cn 空巢老人什么意思hcv7jop5ns4r.cn 度蜜月什么意思hanqikai.com 薄荷泡水喝有什么功效hcv9jop6ns3r.cn 杭盖是什么意思shenchushe.com
百度